Bird vocalization includes both bird calls and bird songs. In non-technical use, bird songs are the bird sounds that are melodious to the human ear. In ornithology and birding, (relatively complex) songs are distinguished by function from (relatively simple) calls.
Definition[]
The distinction between songs and calls is based upon complexity, length, and context. Songs are longer and more complex and are associated with courtship and mating, while calls tend to serve such functions as alarms or keeping members of a flock in contact.[3] Other authorities such as Howell and Webb (1995) make the distinction based on function, so that short vocalizations, such as those of pigeons, and even non-vocal sounds, such as the drumming of woodpeckers and the "winnowing" of snipes' wings in display flight, are considered songs.[4] Still others require song to have syllabic diversity and temporal regularity akin to the repetitive and transformative patterns that define music. It is generally agreed upon in birding and ornithology which sounds are songs and which are calls, and a good field guide will differentiate between the two.
Bird song is best developed in the order Passeriformes. Most song is emitted by male rather than female birds. Song is usually delivered from prominent perches, although some species may sing when flying.
Some groups are nearly voiceless, producing only percussive and rhythmic sounds, such as the storks, which clatter their bills. In some manakins (Pipridae), the males have evolved several mechanisms for mechanical sound production, including mechanisms for stridulation not unlike those found in some insects.[5] The production of sounds by mechanical means as opposed to the use of the syrinx has been termed variously instrumental music by Charles Darwin, mechanical sounds[6] and more recently sonation.[7] The term sonate has been defined as the act of producing non-vocal sounds that are intentionally modulated communicative signals, produced using non-syringeal structures such as the bill, wings, tail, feet and body feathers.[7]
Anatomy[]
The avian vocal organ is called the syrinx; it is a bony structure at the bottom of the trachea (unlike the larynx at the top of the mammalian trachea). The syrinx and sometimes a surrounding air sac resonate to sound waves that are made by membranes past which the bird forces air. The bird controls the pitch by changing the tension on the membranes and controls both pitch and volume by changing the force of exhalation. It can control the two sides of the trachea independently, which is how some species can produce two notes at once.
Function[]
Bird song evolved through sexual selection, and experiments suggest that the quality of bird song may be a good indicator of fitness.[8] Experiments also suggest that parasites and diseases may directly affect song characteristics such as song rate, which thereby act as reliable indicators of health.[9][10] The song repertoire also appears to indicate fitness in some species.[11][12] The ability of male birds to hold and advertise territories using song also demonstrates their fitness.
Communication through bird calls can be between individuals of the same species or even across species. Birds communicate alarm through vocalizations and movements that are specific to the threat, and bird alarms can be understood by other animal species, including other birds, in order to identify and protect against the specific threat.[13] Mobbing calls are used to recruit individuals in an area where an owl or other predator may be present. These calls are characterized by wide-frequency spectra, sharp onset and termination, and repetitiveness that are common across species and are believed to be helpful to other potential "mobbers" by being easy to locate. The alarm calls of most species, on the other hand, are characteristically high-pitched, making the caller difficult to locate.[14]
Individual birds may be sensitive enough to identify each other through their calls. Many birds that nest in colonies can locate their chicks using their calls.[15] Calls are sometimes distinctive enough for individual identification even by human researchers in ecological studies.[16]
Many birds engage in duet calls. In some cases, the duets are so perfectly timed as to appear almost as one call. This kind of calling is termed antiphonal duetting.[17] Such duetting is noted in a wide range of families including quails,[18] bushshrikes,[19] babblers such as the scimitar babblers, some owls[20] and parrots.[21] In territorial songbirds, birds are more likely to countersing when they have been aroused by simulated intrusion into their territory.[22] This implies a role in intraspecies aggressive competition.
Some birds are excellent vocal mimics. In some tropical species, mimics such as the drongos may have a role in the formation of mixed-species foraging flocks.[23] Vocal mimicry can include conspecifics, other species or even man-made sounds. Many hypotheses have been made on the functions of vocal mimicry including suggestions that they may be involved in sexual selection by acting as an indicator of fitness, help brood parasites, or protect against predation, but strong support is lacking for any function.[24] Many birds, especially those that nest in cavities, are known to produce a snakelike hissing sound that may help deter predators at close range.[25]
Some cave-dwelling species, including the Oilbird[26] and swiftlets (Collocalia and Aerodramus spp.),[27] use audible sound (with the majority of sonic location occurring between 2 and 5 kHz[28]) to echolocate in the darkness of caves. The only bird known to make use of infrasound (at about 20 Hz) is the western capercaillie.[29]
The hearing range of birds is from below 50 Hz (infrasound) to above 20 kHz (ultrasound), with maximum sensitivity between 1 and 5 kHz.[12][30] The range of frequencies at which birds call in an environment varies with the quality of habitat and the ambient sounds. It has been suggested that narrow bandwidths, low frequencies, low-frequency modulations, and long elements and inter-element intervals should be found in habitats with complex vegetation structures (which would absorb and muffle sounds), while high frequencies, broad bandwidth, high-frequency modulations (trills), and short elements and inter-elements may be expected in habitats with herbaceous cover.Template:Vague[31][32] It has been hypothesized that the available frequency range is partitioned, and birds call so that overlap between different species in frequency and time is reduced. This idea has been termed the "acoustic niche".[33] Birds sing louder and at a higher pitch in urban areas, where there is ambient low-frequency noise.[34][35]
Learning[]
The songs of different species of birds vary and are generally typical of the species. In modern-day biology, bird song is typically analyzed using a sound spectrograph. Species vary greatly in the complexity of their songs and in the number of distinct kinds of song they sing (up to 3000 in the Brown Thrasher); individuals within some species vary in the same way. In a few species, such as lyrebirds and mockingbirds, songs imbed arbitrary elements learned in the individual's lifetime, a form of mimicry (though maybe better called "appropriation" [Ehrlich et al.], as the bird does not pass for another species). As early as 1773, it was established that birds learned calls, and cross-fostering experiments succeeded in making linnet Acanthis cannabina learn the song of a skylark, Alauda arvensis.[37] In many species, it appears that although the basic song is the same for all members of the species, young birds learn some details of their songs from their fathers, and these variations build up over generations to form dialects.[38]
Song learning in juvenile birds occurs in two stages: sensory learning, which involves the juvenile listening to the father or other conspecific bird and memorizing the spectral and temporal qualities of the song (song template), and sensorimotor learning, which involves the juvenile bird producing its own vocalizations and practicing its song until it accurately matches the memorized song template.[39] During the sensorimotor learning phase, song production begins with highly variable sub-vocalizations called "sub-song", which is akin to babbling in human infants. Soon after, the juvenile song shows certain recognizable characteristics of the imitated adult song, but still lacks the stereotypy of the crystallized song – this is called "plastic song". [40] Finally, after two or three months of song learning and rehearsal (depending on species), the juvenile produces a crystallized song, characterized by spectral and temporal stereotypy (very low variability in syllable production and syllable order).[41] Some birds, such as Zebra Finches, which are the most popular species for birdsong research, have overlapping sensory and sensorimotor learning stages.[36]
Research has indicated that birds' acquisition of song is a form of motor learning that involves regions of the basal ganglia. Further, the PDP has been considered homologous to a mammalian motor pathway originating in the cerebral cortex and descending through the brain stem, while the AFP has been considered homologous to the mammalian cortical pathway through the basal ganglia and thalamus.[40] Models of bird-song motor learning can be useful in developing models for how humans learn speech.[42] In some species such as Zebra Finches, learning of song is limited to the first year; they are termed "age-limited" or "close-ended" learners. Other species such as the canaries can develop new songs even as sexually mature adults; these are termed "open-ended" learners.[43][44]
Researchers have hypothesized that learned songs allow the development of more complex songs through cultural interaction, thus allowing intraspecies dialects that help birds to identify kin and to adapt their songs to different acoustic environments.[45]
Neuroanatomy[]
The acquisition and learning of bird song involves a group of distinct brain areas that are aligned in two connecting pathways [40]:
- Anterior forebrain pathway (vocal learning): composed of Area X, which is a homologue to mammalian basal ganglia; the lateral part of the magnocellular nucleus of anterior neostriatum (LMAN), also considered a part of the avian basal ganglia; and the dorso-lateral division of the medial thalamus (DLM).
- Posterior descending pathway (vocal production): composed of HVC (proper name, although sometimes referred to as the high vocal center); the robust nucleus of the arcopallium (RA); and the tracheosyringeal part of the hypoglossal nucleus (nXIIts).[46][47]
The posterior descending pathway (PDP) is required throughout a bird's life for normal song production, while the anterior forebrain pathway (AFP) is necessary for song learning in juvenilles and plasticity/maintenance in adults, but not for adult song production.[48]
Both neural pathways in the song system begin at the level of HVC, which projects information both to the RA (premotor nucleus) and to Area X of the anterior forebrain. Information in the posterior descending pathway (also referred to as the vocal production or motor pathway) descends from HVC to RA, and then from RA to the tracheosyringeal part of the hypoglossal nerve (nXIIts), which then controls muscular contractions of the syrinx.[40][49]
Information in the anterior forebrain pathway is projected from HVC to Area X (basal ganglia), then from Area X to the DLM (thalamus), and from DLM to LMAN, which then links the vocal learning and vocal production pathways through connections back to the RA. Some investigators have posited a model in which the connection between LMAN and RA carries an instructive signal based on evaluation of auditory feedback (comparing the bird's own song to the memorized song template), which adaptively alters the motor program for song output.[48][50] The generation of this instructive signal could be facilitated by auditory neurons in Area X and LMAN that show selectivity for the temporal qualities of the bird's own song (BOS) and its tutor song, providing a platform for comparing the BOS and the memorized tutor song.[50][51]
Models regarding the real-time error-correction interactions between the AFP and PDP will be considered in the future. Other current research has begun to explore the cellular mechanisms underlying HVC control of temporal patterns of song structure and RA control of syllable production.[52] Brain structures involved in both pathways show sexual dimorphism in many bird species, usually causing males and females to sing differently. Some of the known types of dimorphisms in the brain include the size of nuclei, the number of neurons present, and the number of neurons connecting one nucleus to another.[53] In the extremely dimorphic Zebra Finches (Taeniopygia guttata), a species in which only males typically sing, the size of the HVC and RA are approximately three to six times larger in males than in females, and Area X does not appear to be recognizable in females.[54] Research suggests that exposure to sex steroids during early development is partially responsible for these differences in the brain. Female Zebra Finches treated with estradiol after hatching followed by testosterone or dihydrotestosterone (DHT) treatment in adulthood will develop an RA and HVC similar in size to males and will also display male-like singing behavior.[55] Hormone treatment alone does not seem to produce female finches with brain structures or behavior exactly like males. Furthermore, other research has shown results that contradict what would be expected based on our current knowledge of mammalian sexual differentiation. For example, male Zebra Finches castrated or given sex steroid inhibitors as hatchlings still develop normal masculine singing behavior.[53] This suggests that other factors, such as the activation of genes on the z chromosome, might also play a role in normal male song development.[56]
Hormones also have activational effects on singing and the song nuclei in adult birds. In canaries (Serinus canaria), females normally sing less often and with less complexity than males. However, when adult females are given androgen injections, their singing will increase to an almost male-like frequency.[57] Furthermore, adult females injected with androgens also show an increased size in the HVC and RA regions.[58] Melatonin is another hormone that is also believed to influence song behavior in adults, as many songbirds show melatonin receptors in neurons of the song nuclei.[59] Both the European Starling (Sturnus vulgaris) and House Sparrow (Passer domesticus) have demonstrated changes in song nuclei correlated with differing exposures to darkness and secretions of melatonin.[60][61] This suggests that melatonin might play a role in the seasonal changes of singing behavior in songbirds that live in areas where the amount of daylight varies significantly throughout the year. Several other studies have looked at seasonal changes in the morphology of brain structures within the song system and have found that these changes (adult neurogenesis, gene expression) are dictated by photoperiod, hormonal changes and behavior.[62][63]
The gene FOXP2, defects of which affect both speech production and comprehension of language in humans, becomes highly expressed in Area X during periods of vocal plasticity in both juvenile Zebra Finches and adult canaries.[64]
Auditory feedback in birdsong learning[]
Early experiments by Thorpe in 1954 showed the importance of a bird being able to hear a tutor's song. When birds are raised in isolation, away from the influence of conspecific males, they still sing. While the song they produce, called "isolate song", resembles the song of a wild bird, it shows distinctly different characteristics from the wild song and lacks its complexity.[65] The importance of the bird being able to hear itself sing in the sensorimotor period was later discovered by Konishi. Birds deafened before the song-crystallization period went on to produce songs that were distinctly different from the wild type and islolate song.[66][67] Since the emergence of these findings, investigators have been searching for the neural pathways that facilitate sensory/sensorimotor learning and mediating the matching of the bird's own song with the memorized song template.
Several studies over recent decades have looked at the neural mechanisms underlying birdsong learning by performing lesions to relevant brain structures involved in the production or maintenance of song or by deafening birds before and/or after song crystallization. Another recent experimental approach was recording the bird's song and then playing it back while the bird is singing, causing perturbed auditory feedback (the bird hears the superposition of its own song and a fragmented portion of a previous song syllable).[41] After Nordeen & Nordeen[68] made a landmark discovery as they demonstrated that auditory feedback was necessary for the maintenance of song in adult birds with crystallized song, Leonardo & Konishi (1999) designed an auditory feedback perturbation protocol in order to explore the role of auditory feedback in adult song maintenance further, to investigate how adult songs deteriorate after extended exposure to perturbed auditory feedback, and to examine the degree to which adult birds could recover crystallized song over time after being removed from perturbed feedback exposure. This study offered further support for role of auditory feedback in maintaining adult song stability and demonstrated how adult maintenance of crystallized birdsong is dynamic rather than static.
Brainard & Doupe (2000) posit a model in which LMAN (of the anterior forebrain) plays a primary role in error correction, as it detects differences between the song produced by the bird and its memorized song template and then sends an instructive error signal to structures in the vocal production pathway in order to correct or modify the motor program for song production. In their study, Brainard & Doupe (2000) showed that while deafening adult birds led to the loss of song stereotypy due to altered auditory feedback and non-adaptive modification of the motor program, lesioning LMAN in the anterior forebrain pathway of adult birds that had been deafened led to the stabilization of song (LMAN lesions in deafened birds prevented any further deterioration in syllable production and song structure).
Currently, there are two competing models that elucidate the role of LMAN in generating an instructive error signal and projecting it to the motor production pathway:
- Bird’s Own Song (BOS)-tuned Error Correction Model
- During singing, the activation of LMAN neurons will depend on the match between auditory feedback from the song produced by the bird and the stored song template. If this is true, then the firing rates of LMAN neurons will be sensitive to changes in auditory feedback.
- Efference Copy Model of Error Correction
- An efference copy of the motor command for song production is the basis of the real-time error-correction signal. During singing, activation of LMAN neurons will depend on the motor signal used to generate the song, and the learned prediction of expected auditory feedback based on that motor command. Error correction would occur more rapidly in this model.
Leonardo [69] tested these models directly by recording spike rates in single LMAN neurons of adult Zebra Finches during singing in conditions with normal and perturbed auditory feedback. His results did not support the BOS-tuned error correction model, as the firing rates of LMAN neurons were unaffected by changes in auditory feedback and therefore, the error signal generated by LMAN appeared unrelated to auditory feedback. Moreover, the results from this study supported the predictions of the efference copy model, in which LMAN neurons are activated during singing by the efference copy of the motor signal (and its predictions of expected auditory feedback), allowing the neurons to be more precisely time-locked to changes in auditory feedback.
Mirror neurons and vocal learning[]
A mirror neuron is a neuron that discharges both when an individual performs an action and when he/she perceives that same action being performed by another.[70] These neurons were first discovered in macaque monkeys, but recent research suggests that mirror neuron systems may be present in other animals including humans.[71]
Mirror neurons have the following characteristics:[70]
- They are located the premotor cortex.
- They exhibit both sensory and motor properties.
- They are action-specific – mirror neurons are only active when an individual is performing or observing a certain type of action (e.g., grasping an object).
Because mirror neurons exhibit both sensory and motor activity, some researchers have suggested that mirror neurons may serve to map sensory experience onto motor structures.[72] This has implications for birdsong learning– many birds rely on auditory feedback to acquire and maintain their songs. Mirror neurons may be mediating this comparison of what the bird hears, how it compares to a memorized song template, and what he produces.
In search of these auditory-motor neurons, Jonathan Prather and other researchers at Duke University recorded the activity of single neurons in the HVCs of swamp sparrows.[73] They discovered that the neurons that project from the HVC to Area X (HVCX neurons) are highly responsive when the bird is hearing a playback of his own song. These neurons also fire in similar patterns when the bird is singing that same song. Swamp sparrows employ 3-5 different song types, and the neural activity differs depending on which song is heard or sung. The HVCX neurons only fire in response to the presentation (or singing) of one of the songs, the primary song type. They are also temporally selective, firing at a precise phase in the song syllable.
Prather, et al. found that during the short period of time before and after the bird sings, his HVCX neurons become insensitive to auditory input. In other words, the bird becomes "deaf" to his own song. This suggests that these neurons are producing a corollary discharge, which would allow for direct comparison of motor output and auditory input.[74] This may be the mechanism underlying learning via auditory feedback. These findings are also in line with Leonardo's (2004) efference copy model of error correction in birdsong learning and production.
Overall, the HVCX auditory motor neurons in swamp sparrows are very similar to the visual motor mirror neurons discovered in primates. Like mirror neurons, the HVCX neurons:
- Are located in a premotor brain area
- Exhibit both sensory and motor properties
- Are action-specific – a response is only triggered by the "primary song type"
The function of the mirror neuron system is still unclear. Some scientists speculate that mirror neurons may play a role in understanding the actions of others, imitation, theory of mind and language acquisition, though there is currently insufficient neurophysiological evidence in support of these theories.[72] Specifically regarding birds, it is possible that the mirror neuron system serves as a general mechanism underlying vocal learning, but further research is needed. In addition to the implications for song learning, the mirror neuron system could also play a role in territorial behaviors such as song-type matching and countersinging.[75]
Identification and systematics[]
The specificity of bird calls has been used extensively for species identification. The calls of birds have been described using words or nonsense syllables or line diagrams.[76] Common terms in English include words such as quack, chirp and chirrup. These are subject to imagination and vary greatly; a well-known example is the White-throated Sparrow's song, given in Canada as O sweet Canada Canada Canada and in New England as Old Sam Peabody Peabody Peabody (also Where are you Frederick Frederick Frederick?). In addition to nonsense words, grammatically correct phrases have been constructed as likenesses of the vocalizations of birds. For example, the Barred Owl produces a motif which some bird guides describe as Who cooks for you? Who cooks for you all? with the emphasis placed on you.[77]
The use of spectrograms to visualize bird song was first introduced by W. H. Thorpe.[78][79] These visual representations are also called sonograms or sonagrams. Some recent field guides for birds use sonograms to document the calls and songs of birds.[80] The sonogram is objective, unlike descriptive phrases, but proper interpretation requires experience. Sonograms can also be roughly converted back into sound.[81][82]
Bird song is an integral part of bird courtship and is a pre-zygotic isolation mechanism involved in the process of speciation. Many allopatric sub-species show differences in calls. These differences are sometimes minute, often detectable only in the sonograms. Song differences in addition to other taxonomic attributes have been used in the identification of new species.[83] The use of calls has led to proposals for splitting of species complexes such as those of the Mirafra bushlarks.[84]
Recording[]
Template:Expand section
The first known recording of birdsong was made in 1889 by Ludwig Koch,[85] who went on to become an eminent wildlife recordist and BBC natural history presenter.[85]
Other notable birdsong recordists include Eric Simms, Chris Watson and, in France, Jean-Claude Roché, François Charron, Fernand Deroussen.
Bird language[]
The language of the birds has long been a topic for anecdote and speculation. That calls have meanings that are interpreted by their listeners has been well demonstrated. Domestic chickens have distinctive alarm calls for aerial and ground predators, and they respond to these alarm calls appropriately.[86][87] However, a language has, in addition to words, structures and rules. Studies to demonstrate the existence of language have been difficult due to the range of possible interpretations. Research on parrots by Irene Pepperberg is claimed to demonstrate the innate ability for grammatical structures, including the existence of concepts such as nouns, adjectives and verbs.[88] Studies on starling vocalizations have also suggested that they may have recursive structures.[89]
The term bird language may also more informally refer to patterns in bird vocalizations that communicate information to other birds or other animals in general.[90]
Bird song and music[]
Some musicologists believe that birdsong has had a large influence on the development of music.[91] Although the extent of this influence is impossible to gauge,[92] it is sometimes easy to see some of the specific ways composers have integrated birdsong with music.
There seem to be three general ways musicians or composers can be affected by birdsong: they can be influenced or inspired (consciously or unconsciously) by birdsong, they can include intentional imitations of bird song in a composition, or they can incorporate recordings of birds into their works.
In his book Why Birds Sing, David Rothernberg claims that birds vocalize traditional scales used in human music, such as the pentatonic scale (e.g., Hermit Thrush) and diatonic scale (e.g., Wood Thrush), providing evidence that birdsong not only sounds like music, but is music in the human sense. This claim has been refuted by Sotorrio (Tone Spectra), who has shown that birds are not selecting scale tones from a miriad of tonal possibilities, but are filtering out and reinforcing the available set of overtones from the fundamental tones of their vocal cords. This requires "far less musical intelligence and deliberate appropriation", and in this regard, he suggests birdsong has something in common with Mongolian throat-singing and jaw-harp music. Sotorrio also claims that musicians like Rothernberg are deceived by "a perculiar form of Pareidolia" whereby complex tonal information is reduced to human scale concepts due to a "fixation on music as it is written rather than as it sounds". Rothernberg's claims were expored in the BBC documentary Why Birds Sing.
Compositions that imitate or use birdsong[]
One early example of a composition that imitates birdsong is Janequin's "Le Chant Des Oiseaux", written in the 16th century.
Other composers who have quoted birds or have used birdsong as a compositional springboard include Vivaldi ("Spring" from the Four Seasons)), Biber (Sonata Representativa), Beethoven (Sixth Symphony), Wagner (Siegfried) and the jazz musicians Paul Winter (Flyway) and Jeff Silverbush (Grandma Mickey).[93]
O Urubu e o Gavião
The choro "O Urubu e o Gavião" ("the vulture and the hawk") quotes several birdsongs.
|
|
Problems listening to this file? See media help. |
The twentieth-century French composer Olivier Messiaen composed with birdsong extensively. His Catalogue d'Oiseaux is a seven-book set of solo piano pieces based upon birdsong. His orchestral piece Réveil des Oiseaux is composed almost entirely of birdsong. Many of his other compositions, including Quatuor pour la fin du temps, similarly integrate birdsong.[94]
The Italian composer Ottorino Respighi, with his The Pines of Rome (1923–1924), may have been the first to compose a piece of music that calls for pre-recorded birdsong. A few years later, Respighi wrote Gli Uccelli ("The birds"), based on Baroque pieces imitating birds.
The Finnish composer Einojuhani Rautavaara in 1972 wrote an orchestral piece of music called Cantus Arcticus (Opus 61, dubbed Concerto for Birds and Orchestra), making extensive use of pre-recorded birdsongs from Arctic regions, such as migrating swans.
The American jazz musician Eric Dolphy sometimes listened to birds while he practiced the flute. He claimed to have incorporated bird song into some of his improvisational music.
In the 1960s and 1970s, many rock bands included sound effects in their recordings. Birds were a popular choice. The English band Pink Floyd included bird sound effects in many of the songs from their 1969 albums Soundtrack from the Film More and Ummagumma (for example, "Grantchester Meadows"). Similarly, the English singer Kate Bush incorporated bird sound effects into much of the music on her 2005 album, Aerial.
The Music hall artist Ronnie Ronalde has gained notoriety for his whistling imitations of birds and for integrating birdsong with human song. His songs "In A Monastery Garden" and "If I Were A Blackbird" include imitations of the blackbird, his "signature bird".[citation needed]
The French composer François-Bernard Mâche has been credited with the creation of zoomusicology, the study of the music of animals. His essay "Musique, mythe, nature, ou les Dauphins d'Arion" (1983) includes a study of "ornitho-musicology", in which he speaks of "animal musics" and a longing to connect with nature.
The German DJ, techno music producer and naturalist Dominik Eulberg is an avid bird watcher, and several tracks by him prominently feature sampled bird sounds and even are titled after his favourite specimens.[citation needed]
The productions of The Jewelled Antler Collective often use field recordings featuring birdsong.
In 2007, the CT Collective issed two free albums devoted to music made using bird songs (one with human interaction, one without). The project was co-ordinated by looping musician Nick Robinson.
Other recent composers who have used birdsong in their music include R. Murray Schafer, Michel Gonneville, Hollis Taylor, Kyoko Kobayashi, Rozalie Hirs, Magnus Robb, Stephen Preston and Emily Doolittle.[95]
Bird song and poetry[]
Bird song is a popular subject in poetry. Famous poems inspired by bird song include Percy Bysshe Shelley's "To a Skylark" ("Hail to thee, blithe Spirit!/Bird thou never wert") and Gerard Manley Hopkins' "Sea and Skylark". Birdsongs and their relations to Middle-earth inhabitants are a common motif in J. R. R. Tolkien's literary work.[citation needed] The Grateful Dead performed a song called "Bird Song" that Jerry Garcia wrote and dedicated to Janis Joplin.
See also[]
Template:Colbegin
- Animal communication
- Animal language
- Bioacoustics
- Biomusic
- Biophony
- Cock a doodle doo
- Dawn chorus
- Flight Call
- Language of the birds
- Lateralization of bird song
- Lombard effect
- Talking bird
- Vinkenzetting
- Vocal learning
Template:Colend
References[]
- ^ Sclater, PL (1860). "List of Birds collected by Mr. Fraser in Ecuador, at Nanegal, Calacali, Perucho, and Puellaro, with notes and descriptions of new species". Proc. Zool. Soc. London: 83–97.
- ^ Darwin, Charles (1871). The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. volume 2. John Murray, London. pp. 65–66. ISBN 1-108-00510-1.
- ^ Ehrlich, Paul R., David S. Dobkin, and Darryl Wheye. ""Bird Voices" and "Vocal Development" from Birds of Stanford essays". Retrieved 9-Sep-2008. Check date values in:
|access-date=
(help) - ^ Howell, Steve N. G., and Sophie Webb (1995). A Guide to the Birds of Mexico and Northern Central America. Oxford University Press. ISBN 0-19-854012-4.
- ^ Bostwick, Kimberly S. and Richard O. Prum (2005). "Courting Bird Sings with Stridulating Wing Feathers". Science. 309 (5735): 736. PMID 16051789. doi:10.1126/science.1111701.
- ^ Manson-Barr, P. and Pye, J. D. (1985). Mechanical sounds. In A Dictionary of Birds (ed. B. Campbell and E. Lack), pp. 342-344. Staffordshire: Poyser.
- ^ a b Bostwick, Kimberly S. and Richard O. Prum (2003). "High-speed video analysis of wing-snapping in two manakin clades (Pipridae: Aves)". The Journal of Experimental Biology. 206 (Pt 20): 3693–3706. PMID 12966061. doi:10.1242/jeb.00598.
- ^ Read, A. W. and D. M. Weary (1990). "Sexual selection and the evolution of bird song: A test of the Hamilton-Zuk hypothesis". Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology. 26 (1): 47–56. doi:10.1007/BF00174024.
- ^ Garamszegi, L. Z., A. P. Møller, János Török, Gábor Michl, Péter Péczely and Murielle Richard (2004). "Immune challenge mediates vocal communication in a passerine bird: an experiment". Behavioral Ecology. 15 (1): 148–157. doi:10.1093/beheco/arg108.
- ^ Redpath, S. M., Bridget M Appleby, Steve J Petty (2000). "Do male hoots betray parasite loads in Tawny Owls?". Journal of Avian Biology. 31 (4): 457–462. doi:10.1034/j.1600-048X.2000.310404.x.
- ^ Reid, J. M., Peter Arcese, Alice L. E. V. Cassidy, Sara M. Hiebert, James N. M. Smith, Philip K. Stoddard, Amy B. Marr, and Lukas F. Keller (2005). "Fitness Correlates of Song Repertoire Size in Free-Living Song Sparrows (Melospiza melodia)". The American Naturalist. 165 (3): 299–310. PMID 15729661. doi:10.1086/428299.
- ^ a b Møller AP, J. Erritzøe, L. Z. Garamszegi (2005). "Covariation between brain size and immunity in birds: implications for brain size evolution" (PDF). Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 18 (1): 223–237. PMID 15669979. doi:10.1111/j.1420-9101.2004.00805.x.
- ^ "A Bird's World: Speaking in a Bird's Language". Museum of Science, Boston. 2008.
- ^ Marler, P. (1955). "Characteristics of some animal calls". Nature. 176 (4470): 6–8. doi:10.1038/176006a0.
- ^ Lengagne, T., J. Lauga and T. Aubin (2001). "Intra-syllabic acoustic signatures used by the King Penguin in parent-chick recognition: an experimental approach" (PDF). The Journal of Experimental Biology. 204 (Pt 4): 663–672. PMID 11171348.
- ^ Wayne Delport, Alan C Kemp, J. Willem H Ferguson (2002). "Vocal identification of individual African Wood Owls Strix woodfordii: a technique to monitor long-term adult turnover and residency". Ibis. 144 (1): 30–39. doi:10.1046/j.0019-1019.2001.00019.x.
- ^ Thorpe, W. H. (Antiphonal singing in birds as evidence for avian auditory reaction time). "Antiphonal Singing in Birds as Evidence for Avian Auditory Reaction Time". Nature. 197 (4869): 774–776. doi:10.1038/197774a0. Check date values in:
|date=
(help) - ^ Stokes, A., W. and H. W. Williams (1968). "Antiphonal calling in quail". Auk. 85: 83–89.
- ^ Harris, Tony; Franklin, Kim (2000). Shrikes and Bush-Shrikes. Princeton University Press. pp. 257–260. ISBN 0-691-07036-9. Cite uses deprecated parameter
|coauthors=
(help) - ^ Osmaston, B. B. (1941). ""Duetting" in birds". Ibis. 5 (2): 310–311. doi:10.1111/j.1474-919X.1941.tb00620.x.
- ^ Power, D. M. (1966). "Antiphonal duetting and evidence for auditory reaction time in the Orange-chinned Parakeet". Auk. 83: 314–319.
- ^ Hyman, Jeremy (2003). "Countersinging as a signal of aggression in a territorial songbird" (PDF). Animal Behaviour. 65 (6): 1179–1185. doi:10.1006/anbe.2003.2175.
- ^ Goodale, E. and Kotagama, S. W. (2005). "Testing the roles of species in mixed-species bird flocks of a Sri Lankan rain forest". Journal of Tropical Ecology. 21 (6): 669–676. doi:10.1017/S0266467405002609.
- ^ Kelley, LA, RL Coe, JR Madden & SD Healy (2008). "Vocal mimicry in songbirds". Animal Behaviour. 76 (3): 521–528. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.04.012.
- ^ Marler, Peter; Hans Willem Slabbekoorn (2004). Nature's music: the science of birdsong. Academic Press. p. 145. ISBN 0-12-473070-1. Cite uses deprecated parameter
|coauthor=
(help) - ^ Suthers RA and Hector DH (1985). "The physiology of vocalization by the echolocating Oilbird, Steatornis caripensis". J. Comp. Physiol. 156 (2): 243–266. doi:10.1007/BF00610867.
- ^ Suthers RA and Hector DH (1982). "Mechanism for the production of echolocating clicks by the Grey Swiftlet, Collocalia spodiopygia". J. Comp. Physiol. A. 148 (4): 457–470. doi:10.1007/BF00619784.
- ^ Coles RB, Konishi M and Pettigrew JD (1987). "Hearing and echolocation in the Australian Grey Swiftlet, Collocalia spodiopygia". J. Exp. Biol. 129: 365–371.
- ^ Lieser M, P. Berthold1 and G. A. Manley (2005). "Infrasound in the capercaillie ( Tetrao urogallus )". Journal of Ornithology. 146 (4): 395–398. doi:10.1007/s10336-005-0003-y.
- ^ Dooling, R.J. (1982). Auditory perception in birds. Acoustic Communication in Birds, Vol. 1 (eds D.E. Kroodsma & E.H. Miller). pp. 95–130.
- ^ Boncoraglio, G. and Nicola Saino (2007). "Habitat structure and the evolution of bird song: a meta-analysis of the evidence for the acoustic adaptation hypothesis". Functional Ecology. 21: 134–142. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2435.2006.01207.x.
- ^ Morton, E.S. (1975). "Ecological sources of selection on avian sounds". American Naturalist. 109 (965): 17–34. doi:10.1086/282971.
- ^ Krause, Bernard L. (1993). "The Niche Hypothesis" (PDF). The Soundscape Newsletter. 06.
- ^ Henrik Brumm (2004). "The impact of environmental noise on song amplitude in a territorial bird". Journal of Animal Ecology. 73 (3): 434–440. doi:10.1111/j.0021-8790.2004.00814.x.
- ^ Slabbekoorn, H. and Peet, M. (2003). "Birds sing at a higher pitch in urban noise". Nature. 424 (6946): 267. PMID 12867967. doi:10.1038/424267a.
- ^ a b Brainard, M. S. and Doupe, A. J. (2002). "What songbirds teach us about learning". Nature. 417 (6886): 351–358. PMID 12015616. doi:10.1038/417351a.
- ^ Barrington, D. (1773). "Experiments and observations on the singing of birds". Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. 63: 249–291. doi:10.1098/rstl.1773.0031.
- ^ Marler, P., & M. Tamura (1962). "Song dialects in three populations of the white-crowned sparrow". Condor. 64 (5): 368–377. JSTOR 1365545. doi:10.2307/1365545.
- ^ Konishi, M. (2010). "From central pattern generator to sensory template in the evolution of birdsong". Brain & Language. 15: 18–20. PMID 20955898. doi:10.1016/j.bandl.2010.05.001.
- ^ a b c d Nottebohm,F. (2005). "The Neural Basis of Birdsong". PLoS Biol. 3 (5): 163. PMC 1110917Freely accessible. PMID 15884976. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0030164.
- ^ a b Leonardo, A., & Konishi, M. (1999). "Decrystallization of adult birdsong by perturbation of auditory feedback". Nature. 399 (6735): 466–470. PMID 10365958. doi:10.1038/20933.
- ^ Teramitsu I, Kudo LC, London SE, Geschwind DH, White SA (2004). "Parallel FoxP1 and FoxP2 expression in songbird and human brain predicts functional interaction". J. Neurosci. 24 (13): 3152–63. PMID 15056695. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5589-03.2004.
- ^ Nottebohm, F. (2004). "The road we travelled: discovery, choreography, and significance of brain replaceable neurons". Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1016: 628–658. PMID 15313798. doi:10.1196/annals.1298.027.
- ^ Brenowitz, Eliot A. and Michael D. Beecher (2005). "Song learning in birds: diversity and plasticity, opportunities and challenges" (PDF). Trends in Neurosciences. 28 (3): 127–132. PMID 15749165. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2005.01.004.
- ^ Slater, P. J. B. (1989). "Bird song learning: causes and consequences". Ethol. Ecol. Evol. 1: 19–46. doi:10.1080/08927014.1989.9525529.
- ^ Brainard, M. S. and Doupe, A. J. (2000). "Auditory feedback in learning and maintenance of vocal behavior". Nature Rev. Neurosci. 1 (1): 31–40. PMID 11252766. doi:10.1038/35036205.
- ^ Carew, Thomas J. (2000). Behavioral Neurobiology: The Cellular Organization of Natural Behavior. Sinauer Associates, Inc. ISBN 978-0-87893-092-0.
- ^ a b Kao, M.H., Doupe, A.J., & Brainard, M.S. (2005). "Contributions of an avian basal ganglia-forebrain circuit to real=time modulation of song". Nature. 433: 638–642. doi:10.1038/nature03127.
- ^ Suthers, R. (2004). "How birds sing and why it matters". In Marler, P., & Slabbekoorn, H. Nature's music:The science of birdsong. Academic Press. pp. 272–295. ISBN 0-12-473070-1.
- ^ a b Brainard, M. S. and Doupe, A. J. (2000). "Interruption of a basal ganglia-forebrain circuit prevents plasticity of learned vocalizations". Nature. 404 (6779): 762–766. PMID 10783889. doi:10.1038/35008083.
- ^ Kojima, S., & Doupe, A. (2008). "Neural encoding of auditory temporal context in a songbird basal ganglia nucleus, and its independence of birds' song experience". European Journal of Neuroscience. 25 (5): 1231–1244. PMID 18364039.
- ^ Long, M.A., Jin, D.Z., & Fee, M.S. (2010). "Support for a synaptic chain model of neuronal sequence generation". Nature. 468: 394–399. doi:10.1038/nature09514.
- ^ a b Balthazart, J., and Adkins-Regan (2002). "Sexual differentiation of brain and behavior in birds". Hormones, Brain and Behavior. 4: 223–301.
- ^ Nottebohm, F., and Arnold, A.P. (1976). "Sexual dimorphism in vocal control areas of the songbird brain". Science. 194 (4261): 211–213. doi:10.1126/science.959852.
- ^ Gurney, M.E., and Konishi, M. (1980). "Hormone-induced sexual differentiation of brain and behavior in zebra finches". Science. 208 (4450): 1380–1383. doi:10.1126/science.208.4450.1380.
- ^ Tomaszycki, M.L., Peabody, C., Replogle, K., Clayton, D.F, Tempelman, R.J., and Wade, J. (2009). "Sexual differentiation of the zebra finch song system: potential roles for sex chromosome genes". BMC Neuroscience. 10. doi:10.1186/1471-2202-10-24.
- ^ Leonard, S. L. (1939). "Induction of singing in female canaries by injections of male hormone". Proc. Soc. exp. Biol. 41: 429–436. doi:10.3181/00379727-41-10631.
- ^ Nottebohm, F. (1980). "Testosterone triggers growth of brain vocal control nuclei in adult female canaries". Brain Research. 189 (2): 736. PMID 7370785. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(80)90102-X.
- ^ Ball, G.F., and Balthazart, J. (2002). "Neuroendocrine mechanisms regulating reproductive cycles and reproductive behavior in birds". Hormones, Brain, and Behavior. 2: 649–798.
- ^ Bentley, G.E., Van’t Hof, T.J., and Ball, G.F. (1999). "Seasonal neuroplasticity in the songbird telencephalon: A role for melatonin". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 96 (8): 4674–4679. doi:10.1073/pnas.96.8.4674.
- ^ Cassone, V.M., Bartell, P.A., Earnest D.J., and Kumar, V. (2008). "Duration of melatonin regulates seasonal changes in song control nuclei of the house sparrow, Passer domesticus: Independence from gonads and circadian entrainment". Journal of Biological Rhythms. 23 (1): 49–58. doi:10.1177/0748730407311110.
- ^ Ball,G.F., Auger, C.J., Bernard, D.J., Charlier, T.D., Sartor, J.J., Riters, L.V., & Balthazart, J. (2004). "Seasonal plasticity in the song control system: Multiple brain sites of steroid hormone action and the importance of variation in song behavior". Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1016: 586–610. doi:10.1196/annals.1298.043.
- ^ London, S.E., Replogle, K., & Clayton, D.F. (2009). "Developmental shifts in gene expression in the auditory forebrain during the sensitive period for song learning". Developmental Neurobiology. 69 (7): 436–450. PMID 19360720.
- ^ Scharff C, Haesler S (2005). "An evolutionary perspective on FoxP2: strictly for the birds?". Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 15 (6): 694–703. PMID 16266802. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2005.10.004.
- ^ Thorpe, W. (1954). "The process of song-learning in the chaffinch as studied by means of the sound spectograph". Nature. 173 (4402): 465–469. doi:10.1038/173465a0.
- ^ Konishi, M. (1965). "The role of auditory feedback on the control of vocalization in the white-crowned sparrow". Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie. 22 (7): 770–783. PMID 5874921.
- ^ Marler, P. (1970). "A comparative approach to vocal learning: Song development in the white-crowned sparrows". Journal of Comparative Physiological Psychology. 71: 1–25. doi:10.1037/h0029144.
- ^ Nordeen, K.W., & Nordeen, E.J. (1994). "Auditory feedback is necessary for the maintenance of stereotyped song in adult zebra finches". Behavioral and Neural Biology. 71 (1). doi:10.1016/0163-1047(92)90757-U.
- ^ Leonardo, A. (2004). "Experimental test of error-correction birdsong model". Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 101 (48): 16935–16940.
- ^ a b Rizzolatti G., Craighero L. (2004). "The mirror-neuron system". Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 27: 169–192. PMID 15217330. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230.
- ^ Oberman L. M., Pineda J. A., Ramachandran V. S. (2007). "The human mirror neuron system: A link between action observation and social skills". Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2 (1): 62–66. PMC 2555434Freely accessible. PMID 18985120. doi:10.1093/scan/nsl022.
- ^ a b Dinstein, I., Thomas, C., Behrmann, M., Heeger, D.J. (2008). "A mirror up to nature." Current Biology 18: R13-18.
- ^ Prather J. F., Peters S., Nowicki S., Mooney R. (2008). "Precise auditory-vocal mirroring in neurons for learned vocal communication". Nature. 451 (7176): 305–310. PMID 18202651. doi:10.1038/nature06492.
- ^ Tchernichovski O., Wallman J. (2008). "Behavioral neuroscience: Neurons of imitation". Nature. 451 (7176): 249–250. PMID 18202627. doi:10.1038/451249a.
- ^ Miller G (2008). "Mirror neurons may help songbirds stay in tune". Science. 319 (5861): 269. PMID 18202262. doi:10.1126/science.319.5861.269a.
- ^ Saunders, Aretas A (1951) Guide to Bird Songs. Doubleday and Company
- ^ Sibley, David (2000). The Sibley Guide to Birds. Knopf. ISBN 0-679-45122-6.
- ^ Thorpe, W. H. (1958). "The learning of song patterns by birds, with especial reference to the song of the chaffinch Fringilla coelebs". Ibis. 100 (4): 535–570. doi:10.1111/j.1474-919X.1958.tb07960.x.
- ^ Slater, P. J. B. (2003). "Fifty years of bird song research: a case study in animal behaviour". Animal Behaviour. 65 (4): 633–639. doi:10.1006/anbe.2003.2051.
- ^ Robbins, Chandler S., Bertel Bruun, Herbert S. Zim, Arthur Singer (2001). Birds of North America : A Guide To Field Identification. Golden Guides from St. Martin's Press. ISBN 1-58238-090-2.
- ^ Meijer, P.B.L. (1992). "An Experimental System for Auditory Image Representations". IEEE Transactions on Biomedical Engineering. 39 (2): 112–121. PMID 1612614. doi:10.1109/10.121642.
- ^ US Patent. 20030216649. Audible output sonogram analyzer
- ^ Alström, P. & Ranft, R. (2003). "The use of sounds in avian systematics, and the importance of bird sound archives". Bulletin of the British Ornithologists' Club Supplement. 123A: 114–135.
- ^ Alström, P. (1998). "Taxonomy of the Mirafra assamica complex" (PDF). Forktail. 13: 97–107.
- ^ a b "Archive Pioneers - Ludwig Koch and the Music of Nature". BBC Archives. BBC. 2009-04-15. Retrieved 2 September 2011.
- ^ Collias, N. E. (1987). "The vocal repertoire of the Red Junglefowl: A spectrographic classification and the code of communication". The Condor. 89 (3): 510–524. JSTOR 1368641. doi:10.2307/1368641.
- ^ Evans, C. S., Macedonia, J. M., and Marler, P. (1993). "Effects of apparent size and speed on the response of chickens, Gallus gallus, to computer-generated simulations of aerial predators". Animal Behaviour. 46: 1–11. doi:10.1006/anbe.1993.1156.
- ^ Pepperberg, I.M. (2000). The Alex Studies: Cognitive and Communicative Abilities of Grey Parrots. Harvard University Press.
- ^ Marcus, Gary F. (2006-04-27). "Language: Startling starlings". Nature. 440 (7088): 1117–1118. PMID 16641976. doi:10.1038/4401117a.
- ^ Young, Jon (2008). "Bird Language: Exploring the Language of Nature with Jon Young". OWLink Media.
- ^ Matthew Head (1997). "Birdsong and the Origins of Music". Journal of the Royal Musical Association. 122 (1): 1–23. doi:10.1093/jrma/122.1.1.
- ^ Clark, Suzannah (2001). Music Theory and Natural Order from the Renaissance to the Early Twentieth Century. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-77191-9.
- ^ Jeff SilverbushThis link is dead.
- ^ Griffiths, A Technique for the End of Time (1985)
- ^ http://www.sibetrans.com/trans/a94/crickets-in-the-concert-hall-a-history-of-animals-in-western-music
External links[]
Wikimedia Commons has media related to: http://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/Category:Bird_vocalization |
- Bird Language: Exploring the Language of Nature with Jon Young A blog with stories and tips for learning the patterns in bird vocalizations.
- Large collection of audio bird calls collected in Arizona from Ask A Biologist.
- xeno-canto: Community online database of downloadable bird sounds from around the globe ~95,000 recordings of ~8000 species as of April 2012. See also xeno-canto.
- British Library's archive of bird sounds representing more than 8,000 species.
- Listen to Nature includes article "The Language of Birds"
- Bird songs in movies: an unnatural history Humor piece on soundtrack errors
- How do Birds Sing? The mechanics and anatomy of birdsong production
- Bioacoustic Research Program at the Cornell Lab of Ornithology distributes a number of different free birdsong synthesis & analysis programs.
- Macaulay Library at the Cornell Lab of Ornithology is the world's largest collection of animal sounds and associated video.
- Male shama songs and mimic of sounds
- Audio Pitch Tracer Accurate transcription of clean recordings of bird vocalizations to midi
}}
| below = • Category • Portal • Outline }}
Template:Animal language
Template:NeuroethologyNavbox